Login for members

Vision (Im)Possible

By Michael St. Germain
blog

We depend on our eyes while navigating the retail environment. Without visual clues to orient ourselves, we become confused, frustrated and overwhelmed. In-store signage helps us get to where we want to go.

It also helps retailers to:

  • Lead shoppers to locations where they will make purchases
  • Promote brand awareness
  • Stimulate customer spending by making value propositions visible.

To understand how in-store signage works, researchers frequently observe and interview shoppers. They look at sales data and even walk with customers who are willing to narrate as they shop. Swedish researchers Tobias Otterbring, Erik Wastlund, Anders Gustafsson and Poja Shams undertook a field study to investigate how instore signage affects the visual attention and decision-making processes of shoppers. Eye-tracking devices, worn by volunteer shoppers, enabled Otterbring and his colleagues to quantify the amount of visual attention directed at in-store signage.

The team conducted two experiments that:

  • Took place at the same food market
  • Utilized test subjects recruited on-site
  • Measured eye-fixations

Experiment 1 investigated how store-familiar and store unfamiliar customers use in-store signs to navigate and make decisions. One hundred and one volunteer test subjects were recruited while shopping in a market that functioned as the experiment’s setting. Unbeknownst to test subjects, thirty in-store signs were installed for the experiment. Half provided navigational clues, such as advertisements or information regarding nearby goods (figure 1).

The other half provided decision-making information, such as discounts or product details (figure 2).

Test subjects were told that the study they were participating in would investigate visual attention and how it relates to shopping. Those undertaking the 15-minute experiment were fitted with eye-tracking and given a list of six items to retrieve. Test subjects were told that they would not have to pay for the items they selected 3] and were encouraged to act just as they would on any other shopping trip. Test subjects were free to choose whatever products they wanted, as long as they were on their shopping list. Shopping commenced at the entrance to the store. After retrieving their six items, test subjects had their eye-tracking devices removed. Then they filled out a questionnaire asking about their demographic information, store familiarity and navigational ability.

Experiment 1 revealed that:

  • Store-unfamiliar shoppers rely on navigational signs more than store-familiar shoppers do
  • Store–familiar shoppers read decision-making signs more than store-unfamiliar customers do

Researchers believe that, even with the assistance of in-store navigational signs, store-unfamiliar shoppers expend most of their energy searching for products. By the time they find what they are looking for, they are too tired to read more signs. On the other hand, by knowing where their intended purchases are located, store- familiar customers save energy, which they can use to read signs at the store shelf. It follows that store managers, who desire to influence store-unfamiliar customers during the decision-making process, are fighting an uphill battle. Any signage they produce with this objective in mind should be simple, bold and “to-the-point.” Ideally, their signs should also offer a tempting value proposition.

Priming is a technique which exposes a subject to a stimulus that is intended to influence future behavior. For example, imagine that it is hot out; you walk in a store and the first thing you see is a giant sign advertising ice-cream. Experiment 2 investigated how exposure to instore signage priming influences subsequent purchase decisions. Test subjects were recruited on-site and told that they would be participating in an experiment designed to explore visual attention, customer behavior and choice. The prime in experiment 2 was a sign displaying a photo of a muesli product. The sign was positioned next to the actual product and similar, closely priced substitutes.

Test subjects were exposed to the prime from afar. They were then instructed to walk over to the muesli shelf, select a box and deliver it to a researcher. After completing this task, test subjects returned their eyetracking glasses and filled out a questionnaire concerning shopping habits, product usage and demographics.

The results of Experiment 2 suggest that:

  • After customers view in-store signage primes, they will look at visually similar products quicker and more frequently than non-similar products
  • Priming by in-store signage may stimulate remembrance, but this in and of itself, won’t motivate a customer to buy

The researchers believe that in-store signage primes may be more likely to motivate customers if they offer a value proposition. As this was not explored in the experiment, it warrants further investigation. In closing, I share personal communication which I had with Tobias Otterbring, one of the researchers involved in the studies above. In this passage, he discusses future research possibilities… “The structured shopping-list procedure in experiment 1 only asked participants to collect the items on the shopping list, which, by implication, precluded us from studying phenomena such as unplanned purchases.

Therefore, one interesting suggestion for future research would be to investigate whether the specificity of the task at hand influences customers’ subsequent visual attention and choice behavior. Will an initial shopping task with a high (vs. low) level of detail lead to more or less visual attention being directed toward various instore cues at a subsequent shopping task?” These are the issues to investigate next.

This article was published in Insights Magazine, the only business publication focused on neuromarketing. Want to enjoy more excellent neuromarketing reading? Join the NMSBA at www.nmsba.com/join

Close